home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
-
- ==Phrack Inc.==
-
- Volume Three, Issue 30, File #9 of 12
- ___________________________________
- | |
- | The Truth About Lie Detectors |
- |_______ _______|
- | by Razor's Edge |
- | |
- | November 10, 1989 |
- |___________________|
-
- Americans love gadgets, so it is not hard to explain the popularity of the lie
- detector. Many people believe in the validity of lie detectors because the
- instruments and printouts resemble those used by doctors and others who collect
- scientific data and because lie detectors are simple, convenient shortcuts to
- hard complicated decisions. Polygraphy is fast becoming an American obsession
- -- an obsession, incidentally, not shared by the British or the Europeans or,
- as far as we know, the Russians.
-
- American industry's increasing dependence on the polygraph reflects an enormous
- faith in the rational processes of science. Each of us can recall a time when
- our voices sounded funny as we told a lie. Surely, if we can "hear" a lie,
- science can detect one. It comes as a disturbing shock, therefore, to learn
- how fragile the polygraph's scientific foundations really are.
-
- The roots of the lie detector, more formally known as the polygraph, go back to
- the turn of the century, when infatuation with the newly discovered powers of
- electricity more than once overcame common sense. But whereas electric hair
- restorers and high-voltage cancer cures have all but vanished, the polygraph
- persists and even flourishes. According to the best estimates, over one
- million polygraph examinations are administered each year in the united States.
- They are used in criminal investigations, during government security checks,
- and increasingly by nervous employers -- particularly banks and stores. In
- certain parts of the country, a woman must pass a lie detector test before the
- authorities will prosecute a rape. In 1983 the television show Lie Detector
- added the dimension of home entertainment to polygraph tests.
-
- The National Security Agency (NSA) leads the roster of federal polygraph users;
- both it and the CIA rely heavily on polygraph testing for pre-employment and
- routine security screening. The NSA reported giving nearly 10,000 tests in
- 1982 (CIA numbers are classified). Those who are labeled "deceptive" often
- lose their jobs, even if there is no actual evidence against them. Moreover,
- the polygraph report may become a permanent part of an employee's records, and
- it will be extremely difficult to compel a correction.
-
- With the arrest in June 1985 of four Navy men on espionage charges, the issue
- of using polygraphs to uncover spies or ferret out dishonest job seekers has
- come to the forefront of the debate about what should be done to stem the loss
- of defense and company secrets and to dispel potential thieves in the
- workplace.
-
- Much the same issue is at the heart of the protracted wrangle between the
- Reagan Administration and Congress over plans for expanded government use of
- the polygraph. An executive order issued on March 11, 1983, known as National
- Security Decision Directive 84, would have sanctioned for the first time
- "adverse consequences" for a federal employee who refuses to take a test when
- asked. The directive authorized tests to investigate candidates for certain
- security clearances and to ask any federal employee about leaks of classified
- information. (This directive was issued shortly after Reagan's comment about
- being "up to my keister" in press leads.) Almost simultaneously the Department
- of Defense (DOD) released a draft regulation that authorized use of the
- polygraph to screen employees who take on sensitive intelligence assignments;
- it, too, prescribed adverse consequences for refusal.
-
- Critics of the polygraph maintain that its use represents an invasion of
- privacy, especially when the coercive power of the government or an employer is
- behind the application. It is hard for a job applicant to say no when a
- prospective employer asks him or her to take a polygraph test; once hooked up
- to the machine, the applicant may face questions not only about past criminal
- activity but also about matters that an employer may have no business intruding
- upon, such as sexual practices or gambling -- questions asked ostensibly to
- assess the applicant's "character." As a result of such abuses, nineteen
- states and the District of Columbia have made it illegal for an organization to
- ask its employees to take polygraph examinations.
-
- A question more basic than whether the polygraph is an unacceptable invasion of
- privacy is, of course, whether it works. Seeking an answer in the scientific
- literature can be a bewildering experience. A report by the Office of
- Technology Assessment (OTA), commissioned in 1983 by Brooks's Committee on
- Government Operations, summed up the problem by citing twenty-four studies that
- found correct detection of guilt ranging from 35% to 100%.
-
- Polygraph theory thrives on a sort of Pinocchio vision of lying, in which
- physiological reactions -- changes in blood pressure or rate of breathing or
- sweating of the palms -- elicited by a set of questions will reliably betray
- falsehood. Lying, goes the rationale, is deliberate, and the knowledge and
- effort associated with it will make a person upset enough to display a physical
- reaction like a speedup of the heartbeat. The variables measured usually
- include the galvanic skin response (GSR), blood pressure, abdominal
- respiration, and thoracic respiration. The GSR is measured by fingertip
- electrodes that produce changes in the electrical resistance in the palms when
- they are sweating. The blood pressure and pulse are monitored through a system
- that uses a sphygmomanometer cuff, which is usually attached to the biceps
- (this is similar to the way doctors measure blood pressure). There is no
- "specific lie response." The polygraph merely records general emotional
- arousal. It does not distinguish anxiety or indignation from guilt. The real
- "lie detector" is the operator, who interprets the various body responses on
- the machine's output.
-
- Polygraphers claim that it is the form and mix of questions that are the keys
- to their success. The standard format, known as the Control Question Test,
- involves interspersing "relevant" questions with "control" questions. Relevant
- questions relate directly to the critical matter: "Did you participate in the
- robbery of the First National Bank on September 11, 1981?" Control questions,
- on the other hand, are less precise: "In the last twenty years, have you ever
- taken something that did not belong to you?"
-
- In the pretest interview, the polygrapher reviews all the questions and frames
- the control questions to produce "no" answers. It is in this crucial pretest
- phase that the polygrapher's deception comes into play, for he wants the
- innocent subject to dissemble while answering the control questions during the
- actual test.
-
- The assumption underlying the Control Question Test is that the truthful
- subject will display a stronger physiological reaction to the control
- questions, whereas a deceptive subject will react more strongly to the relevant
- questions. That is the heart of it. Modern lie detection relies on nothing
- more than subtle psychological techniques, crude physiological indicators, and
- skilled questioning and interpretation of the results.
-
- Critics claim that polygraphy fails to take the complexities of lying into
- account. For some people lying can be satisfying, fulfilling, exciting, and
- even humorous, depending on their reasons for lying. Other people feel little
- or no emotion when lying. Still others believe their lies and think they are
- telling the truth when they are not. Moreover, the theory holds that deception
- produces distinctive physiological changes that characterize lying and only
- lying. This notion has no empirical support. Quite the contrary: Lying
- produces no known distinctive pattern of physiological activity.
-
- Undeniably, when being dishonest, people can feel great turmoil and a polygraph
- can measure this turmoil. But when apprehensive about being interrogated, they
- can give a similar emotional reaction: When they think they are losing the
- chance for job openings or their jobs are on the line, when they reflect on the
- judgements that could be made about their answers, or, for that matter, when
- they are angry, puzzled, or even amused by the impertinent probing of a total
- stranger. Some control questions may make a person appear guilty. Such
- questions may force a subject into a minor lie or ask about an invented crime
- that nonetheless makes the subject nervous.
-
- Lie detectors are especially unreliable for truthful people. Many more
- innocent people test as "deceptive" than guilty people test as "innocent."
- Those who run a special risk include people who get upset if someone accuses
- them of something they didn't do, people with short tempers, people who tend to
- feel guilty anyway, and people not accustomed to having their word questioned.
- All of these feelings can change heart rate, breathing, and perspiration and
- their heightened feelings are easily confused with guilt.
-
- It has also been shown that polygraphs are easily manipulated. Four hundred
- milligrams of the tranquilizer meprobamate taken an hour or two before a
- polygraph session can make it virtually impossible to spot a liar by his
- physiological responses. In fact, some researchers even argue that an examinee
- can use simple countermeasures, such as biting one's tongue, gouging oneself
- with a fingernail, or stepping on a nail concealed in a shoe, to fake a strong
- reaction to the control questions, thus "beating" the test. According to one
- researcher, one prison inmate, who became the jail-house polygraph expert after
- studying the literature, trained twenty-seven fellow inmates in the seat
- techniques; twenty-three beat the polygraph tests used tons investigate
- violations of prison rules. However, do not try sighing, coughing, or
- clenching your fist or arm. Polygraphers usually are suspicious of those
- techniques and may label you "deceptive" for that reason alone.
-
- It should be obvious that the interpretation of the results of any polygraph
- test will certainly be very difficult. Also, not all responses on the machine
- will agree. What are the present qualifications for a polygrapher? Most of
- the twenty-five or more schools that train examiners provide only an eight-week
- course of instruction and require two years of college for admission. This is
- about one-sixth the study time of the average barber college. Perhaps as many
- as a dozendy time of contemporary polygraphers do hold Ph.D's, but the vast
- majority of the 4,000 to 8,000 practicing examiners had no simple significant
- training in physiology or in psychology, even though lie detection demands
- extremely subtle and difficult psychophysiological interpretations. There are
- no licensing standards for polygraph operators, and, with so many poorly, who
- trained operators, thousands of tests are conducted hastily and haphazardly,
- resulting in highly questionable accuracy. For many innocent people, their
- judge and jury are these unskilled operators.
-
- Honesty is also difficult to predict because it tends to be situation-
- specific. Therefore, it is more dependent on motivation and opportunity than
- on some personality trait. As Bertrand Russell once said, "Virtue is dictated
- by results of circumstance."
-
- Proponents of the polygraph sometimes cite "correct guilty detections": The
- percentage of guilty subjects who are caught by the polygraph. This figure can
- be very impressive: In one study that does not suffer from the failings
- already mentioned, it was 98% correct. But the same study found that 55% of
- innocent subjects were also diagnosed as "deceptive." The handful of studies
- that used a truly random selection of cases and scored them blind produced
- similar results: Overall, 83% of guilty subjects were diagnosed as
- "deceptive," as were 43% of innocent subjects. It's no trick to push the rate
- of correct guilty detections to 100% -- just call everyone "deceptive." You
- don't even need a machine to do that!
-
- Nature published its conclusions last year. Their aggregated findings were
- based on the polygraph charts of 207 criminal suspects, which 14 polygraphers
- scored independently. On the average, they erroneously diagnosed 43% of
- innocent suspects as deceptive. Such errors, called false positives, ranged as
- high as 50%. The corresponding errors of deceptive persons "passing the test,"
- or false negatives, were as high as 36%.
-
- The accuracy rates of "failed" and "passed" depend, of course, on the
- proportion of dishonest persons in the group tested. Thus, if 800 of 1,000
- persons tested are truthful, a test that is 72% accurate overall will accuse
- 144 liars and 224 truthful persons. This is not an impressive accuracy record.
-
- These numbers suggest that the polygraph test is biased against innocent
- people. The problem is accentuated when the test is used in the screening
- situations envisioned in the Reagan Administration proposals (and already
- established at the NSA and the CIA). Everyone is tested, but presumably only a
- very small proportion has done anything wrong. If we assume that one employee
- in a hundred is a spy (probably a gross overestimate), and if we use the 83%
- correct-guilty-detection rate, we find that 51 innocent persons will flunk the
- polygraph test for every real spy who flunks. Any test, whether it is for
- truth or for cancer, has to be extremely accurate to detect a rare phenomenon
- without setting off a lot of false alarms in the process. Even if the test
- were 99% accurate for both guilty and innocent detections, one innocent person
- would be falsely branded for each spy caught. Because of this "case rate"
- problem, the FBI forbids the use of polygraph dragnets: The tests can be used
- only after an initial investigation has narrowed the field of suspects.
-
- Given all the doubts about their validity, why does the government persist in
- using polygraph tests? Some clues are found in the DOD 1983 report on
- polygraph testing -- even in its title, "The Accuracy and Utility of Polygraph
- Testing" which suggests that accuracy and utility are two different things. The
- most that report concludes about accuracy is that it is "significantly above
- chance." Utility, however, is quite another matter. Perhaps the most telling
- statement about lie detectors comes from former president Nixon, who declared
- on one of the White House tapes, "I don't know anything about lie detectors
- other than they scare the hell out of people."
- _______________________________________________________________________________
-