home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
- Xref: sparky sci.skeptic:21433 alt.folklore.urban:31380 alt.atheism:23818 alt.feminism:6133
- Path: sparky!uunet!elroy.jpl.nasa.gov!ncar!noao!arizona.edu!skyblu.ccit.arizona.edu!lippard
- Newsgroups: sci.skeptic,alt.folklore.urban,alt.atheism,alt.feminism
- Subject: Re: Hypnosis and Repression (was Ritual Abuse Taskforce....)
- Message-ID: <16DEC199212084452@skyblu.ccit.arizona.edu>
- From: lippard@skyblu.ccit.arizona.edu (James J. Lippard)
- Date: 16 Dec 1992 12:08 MST
- References: <2933430839.2.p00168@psilink.com>
- Distribution: world,local
- Organization: University of Arizona
- Nntp-Posting-Host: skyblu.ccit.arizona.edu
- News-Software: VAX/VMS VNEWS 1.41
- Lines: 439
-
- In article <2933430839.2.p00168@psilink.com>, p00168@psilink.com (James F. Tims) writes...
- >[text deleted]
- >i.e. the memories are alread hidden. At the very least, "cryptoamnesia"
- >is redundant. Hiding something which is lost falls right in with
- >perceiving things which are below the limits of perception, the
- >ever-popular, self-contradictory "subliminal perception".
-
- But there's more than one definition of "perception," or at least more than
- one way of measuring successful perception, and these different ways of
- measuring give different results. There are plenty of experiments in
- psycholinguistics (e.g., any experiment with masked primes) in which
- subjects claim not to have seen any words but their subsequent behavior
- in tasks such as word completion or reaction times in naming or lexical
- decision show that the masked stimulus was perceived.
-
- >> Another book by Baker, also from Prometheus (1991):
- >>
- >> They Call it Hypnosis
- >>
- >> Voted "Oustanding Academic Book for 1991" - Choice
- >>
- >> In this definitive study Baker traces the history and current status
- >> of the hypnotic concept, explaining what hypnosis is and is not,
- >> what it can and cannot accomplish, as well as its uses, misuses, and
- >> abuses.
- >>
- >> 313 pages - ISBN 0-87975-576-8 $23.93.
-
- >The (Great, Amazing, Fantastic?) Kreskin once claimed that hypnosis
- >doesn't even exist. Does Dr. Baker reach a similar conclusion?
-
- Sort of. See for yourself (my review of his book is followed by
- a reply from Baker):
-
- From _The Arizona Skeptic_, vol. 5, no. 1, July/August 1991,
- pp. 6-7. Copyright (c) 1991 by the Phoenix Skeptics.
- Book Review
- _They Call It Hypnosis_ by Robert A. Baker
- 1990, Prometheus Books, 313pp.
- Reviewed by Jim Lippard
- Robert Baker has written an entertaining and useful book for those
- interested in the facts about hypnosis. While he argues for a
- particular interpretation of hypnosis (the social-psychological
- interpretation favored by researchers such as Spanos, Barber, and
- others), he also presents numerous other interpretations which
- have been offered. On the question of whether hypnosis is a
- special state of consciousness or not, Baker comes down firmly
- (and rightly, in my opinion) on the side of the non-state
- theorists.
- This is a position which contradicts popular culture's view
- of hypnosis, which is how Baker begins his book. He gives
- examples from literature and the mass media of what hypnosis is,
- and then shows how and why they are mistaken. Baker's book then
- gives a history of the concept of hypnosis and a summary of
- contemporary views. An entire chapter is devoted to hypnosis and
- pain.
- The book deals with nearly every major issue in hypnotic
- behavior, though there were a number of subjects which I thought
- could have been dealt with in more detail. For example, Baker
- maintains that "hypnotized" individuals will not do anything they
- would not ordinarily do. To explain such cases as experiments in
- which subjects threw acid at the face of an experimenter (who was
- actually shielded by glass), Baker maintains that in such cases
- the subject knows that the experimenter is taking responsibility
- for his behavior and assumes that nothing will really go wrong
- (pp. 49, 154). This explanation, however, doesn't work for cases
- such as two legal cases from Germany described in Leo Katz's book
- _Bad Acts and Guilty Minds_ (1987, University of Chicago Press,
- pp. 128-133). Katz describes cases where unethical hypnotists
- induced patients to give them large sums of money, commit crimes,
- and attempt murder and suicide. It is perhaps because of cases
- like this that the Model Penal Code (MPC) lists "conduct during
- hypnosis or resulting from hypnotic suggestion" as behaviors which
- are "not voluntary acts." When I asked Baker about these cases,
- he found the MPC definition unreasonable and stated that if the
- descriptions in Katz's book were correct, the people were
- effectively using the hypnosis as an excuse for behavior in which
- they would have engaged anyway. (It is worth noting that the
- alleged hypnosis-induced murder attempts were stopped by the
- subject at the last minute rather than failing for chance reasons,
- and that Katz himself (p. 133) warns that the accounts are
- questionable for the same reason Baker gave me.) This
- explanation, however, does not seem to be subject to scientific
- examination.
- Another story that appears to lend credence to the idea that
- hypnosis can result in loss of voluntary control is found in
- Richard Feynman's autobiography, _Surely You're Joking_, Mr.
- Feynman (1985, W.W. Norton, pp. 53-55). Feynman describes
- volunteering to be hypnotized by a stage hypnotist while a
- graduate student at Princeton. He mentions doing things he
- "couldn't normally do" (a statement Baker does a good job of
- falsifying) and being given the suggestion to walk all the way
- around the room rather than returning to his seat directly.
- Feynman decided to try to resist the suggestion, without success.
- The social-psychological interpretation would state that this is
- simply due to social pressures, not to any magical power of
- hypnosis.
- Baker maintains in his book that there are no differences in
- the EEGs of hypnotized versus non-hypnotized individuals, however
- David Spiegel of Stanford University, a hypnosis researcher,
- maintains otherwise (e.g., Spiegel, Cutcomb, Ren, and Pribram
- 1985). (Nicholas Spanos, in his author's response to commentary
- by Spiegel citing this research (1986, p. 492), argues that
- Spiegel has misinterpreted his data given the nature of the
- control subjects used.) It would have been nice to at least have
- seen some acknowledgment of disagreement on this subject, but
- Spiegel is not even mentioned.
- Another peculiarity of Baker's position on hypnosis was
- pointed out by Stanford hypnosis researcher Ernest Hilgard at the
- session on hypnosis at the 1991 CSICOP conference in Berkeley,
- California. Hilgard noted that Baker rejects the usefulness of
- hypnotic susceptibility scales ("To my dismay I soon discovered
- this sort of screening was of no value ... Neither I nor my fellow
- researchers found the tests to be discriminatory. ... we found
- that nearly all of our subjects scored almost exactly alike--near
- the top--on the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scales, forms A,
- B, and C," p. 35). Hilgard stated that Baker's position is
- contrary to that of not only state theorists, but to non-state
- theorists like Nicholas Spanos. (Indeed, Spanos presented data at
- the same conference which made use of differential results on
- hypnotic susceptibility scales.)
- Finally, the book is somewhat marred by a large number of
- typographical errors which should have been caught by an editor.
- These include not only misspellings (like "Hildgard" for "Hilgard"
- on p. 107), but disagreement in number between verb and subject
- ("Barber's own personal experiences with pain has led him to be
- able to control it," p. 100) and other mistakes. I must say,
- however, that despite these minor flaws, this remains one of the
- best books on the subject of hypnosis I have read. I recommend it
- highly.
-
- References
- Spanos, N. (1986) "Hypnotic Behavior: A Social-Psychological
- Interpretation of Amnesia, Analgesia, and 'Trance Logic'."
- _Behavioral and Brain Sciences_ 9:449-502.
- Spiegel, D., Cutcomb, S., Ren, C., and Pribram, K. (1985)
- "Hypnotic Hallucination Alters Evoked Potentials," _Journal
- of Abnormal Psychology_ 94:249-255.
-
- From _The Arizona Skeptic_ vol. 5, no. 2, September/October 1991,
- p. 7. Copyright (c) 1991 by the Phoenix Skeptics.
- Hypnosis and Free Will
- By Jim Lippard
- Most hypnosis researchers maintain that hypnotized persons cannot
- be induced to do anything contrary to their own personal moral
- code. At least one article in the scientific literature denies
- this claim (Loyd W. Rowland, "Will Hypnotized Persons Try To Harm
- Themselves or Others?", _Journal of Abnormal and Social
- Psychology_ 34(1939):114-117, described in William Corliss' _The
- Unfathomed Mind: A Handbook of Unusual Mental Phenomena_, pp. 120-
- 123). These experiments involved subjects sticking their hands
- into a box containing a rattlesnake (which was actually fake) or
- throwing acid into the face of an experimenter (who was behind an
- unseen panel of glass). A possible rejoinder to this experiment
- is the same as a criticism made of Stanley Milgram's "obedience to
- authority" experiments, where subjects believed they were
- assisting in a psychological experiment by giving painful electric
- shocks to another test subject (actually a simulating assistant of
- the experimenter). The response is to say that the experimental
- situation was one in which the subject had complete trust in the
- experimenter and put all responsibility into his hands. But is
- there any reason to believe that this effect is limited to the
- experimental laboratory?
- There are also, however, a number of reported cases of
- criminal actions being performed by hypnotized persons. For
- example, Leo Katz's book _Bad Acts and Guilty Minds_ (1987,
- University of Chicago Press, pp. 128-133) describes legal cases
- from Germany where unethical hypnotists induced patients to give
- them large sums of money, commit crimes, and attempt murder and
- suicide (the latter two failed). One response to this is to claim
- that the defendants were simply using hypnosis as an excuse to
- avoid prosecution, that they wouldn't have done what they did if
- they had not already been predisposed to do so (this response,
- like the one above to the experiments, is made by Robert A. Baker-
- -see my review of his book _They Call It Hypnosis_, in _The
- Arizona Skeptic_, July/August 1991).
- The latest issue of the _Fortean Times_ (#58, July 1991)
- reports the prosecution of a 57-year-old man, Nelson Nelson, who
- sexually assaulted at least 113 women, preceded by hypnosis. In
- Michael Goss' article, "The Eyes Have It," he reports that most of
- the women assaulted by Nelson over a 25-year period did not report
- it and were only discovered because Nelson kept a diary of his
- exploits. The only source cited for this, however, is "daily
- papers for 2 May 1991." No newspapers, no locations are cited
- (the author lives in Essex, England). Goss also reports a
- psychiatrist, Clifford Salter, whose medical license was revoked
- in 1982 for abusing women under hypnosis. (Again, no sources, and
- the details are too sketchy to know whether hypnosis really played
- a significant role at all. After all, Salter was allegedly
- caught.)
-
- From _The Arizona Skeptic_ vol. 5, no. 4, January/February 1992,
- pp. 2-4. Copyright (c) 1992 by the Phoenix Skeptics.
- Comments on Lippard's Review of _They Call It Hypnosis_
- By Robert A. Baker
- As I tried to make clear in my book, _They Call It Hypnosis_,
- nearly everything about the concept of "hypnosis"* is
- controversial. My primary motive in writing the book was to
- provide the general public with a solid path of reliable
- information through a veritable wilderness of claims and
- counterclaims. Just about everything possible and impossible has
- been claimed about hypnosis. For example, people claimed one
- could hypnotize people behind their backs when they were unaware,
- one could hypnotize people via ESP or over the telephone, people
- can be kept in a trance for seven years or more, and so on and so
- on. In the past, most practitioners of Mesmerism sincerely
- believed that hypnosis gave people supernatural powers, i.e., made
- them clairvoyant, provided them with ESP powers, enabled them to
- communicate with the dead and discarnate spirits, etc. All such
- occult claims have been shown again and again to be unfounded and
- either delusionary or fraudulent or the result of human error.
- [* In Baker's manuscript, every occurrence of this term in
- all its forms appears in quotation marks, and he notes that he has
- done so "to indicate that no such phenomenon exists." I have
- omitted them throughout the rest of the article simply to conserve
- space. --Editor]
- With regard to several of the issues that Lippard felt should
- have been dealt with in more detail let me add a few clarifying
- remarks. First, with regard to the issue of controlling
- hypnotized people or having them carry out behavior of any sort or
- criminal acts against their will--time and again carefully
- controlled experiments have shown that the so-called "hypnotized"
- individual will not do anything he or she would not do when they
- are wide awake. Every ardent young male in the country wishes
- this weren't true. Think about it. All one would have to do is
- learn hypnosis and then he could have his way with all the girls.
- Fortunately for the ladies, this can't be done. No young lady is
- going to surrender under hypnosis any more readily than she would
- surrender if she were wide awake. If she wants to surrender then
- she can, of course, use hypnosis as a handy rationalization. The
- most convincing proof, however, of the fact that people who are
- hypnotized are not robots or automatons under the control of the
- hypnotist comes from the efforts of the CIA, who carried out over
- a decade of research to determine if it was possible to create a
- "Manchurian Candidate"--i.e., to use hypnosis to program a man to
- turn, after the appropriate signal, into a mindless robot killer.
- All of the CIA's efforts proved to be impossible and, as reported
- by Thomas (1989), some of their efforts resembled a Marx Brothers'
- comedy. Hoping to create a "sleeper-killer" who would be used to
- assassinate Castro, the CIA recruited several Cuban refugees from
- Miami and selected one who appeared to the hypnotic experts as the
- "ideal" subject. After days of careful programming and implanting
- the secret word in the subject's unconscious while under hypnosis,
- the day of the final test arrived.
- According to the program, when the Cuban heard the key word
- in the presence of Fidel this would cause him to draw his weapon
- and shoot the dictator. To test the training the hypnotist
- ordered the Cuban to imagine he was at Castro's side. Then the
- hypnotist uttered the key word. Nothing happened. The hypnotist
- tried again and again nothing happened. Finally the hypnotist
- gave up and brought the Cuban out of the trance. Once more the
- hypnotist uttered the secret word--"cigar." This time the man
- looked at the hypnotist blankly and said, "No thanks, I don't
- smoke." Unfortunately, despite the CIA fiasco, the legend of the
- Manchurian Candidate refuses to die.
- As for the matter of Spiegel's findings of differences in the
- brain waves of hypnotized and non-hypnotized individuals (1985),
- to my knowledge these results have not been replicated since.
- Moreover, David Spiegel and his father are strong proponents of
- the "state" theory and both support the notion that
- hypnotizability is a hereditary trait and that an S's ability to
- roll his eyeballs is a clue to his hypnotizability. Spiegel has
- also argued that specific and unique EEG changes accompany the
- personality shifts in MPD** patients and suggests that the
- secondary personalities in MPD cases are biologically independent
- of each other as well as psychologically independent. It should
- be remembered, however, that no EEG differences could be found
- between the three personalities in _The Three Faces of Eve_
- (Thigpen and Cleckley, 1957). Of even greater interest is the
- curious fact that EEG changes can be produced by people
- _simulating_ multiple personality (Coons, Milstein, and Marley,
- 1982) which, again, suggests the social-cognitive role-playing
- nature of many MPD disorders as well as so-called "hypnosis."
- Most experts in the EEG area have concluded that no differences in
- EEG patterns can be found between the hypnotized and unhypnotized
- states. In the words of Negley-Parker (1986), "There is the
- possibility that differences between the brain waves of the
- hypnotized and 'awake' subjects are too subtle to be picked up by
- the relatively crude measurements of the electroencephalogram, but
- the available evidence indicates that brain waves are practically
- the same in hypnosis or out of it." (p. 9)
- [** Multiple Personality Disorder. --Editor]
- This conclusion is also shared by Paul Davies (1988) in his
- review of the available evidence at the time. We should remember,
- however, that hypnosis does correlate with relaxation and that
- relaxation _per se_ can produce a number of marked physiological
- and EEG changes. We must never forget that the EEG is still--even
- today--a very crude and unreliable tool. Further, we must also
- remember that experimenter bias and expectation is such that we
- usually pretty much find whatever it is we are looking for. Until
- additional replications of Spiegel's work appear, I remain quite
- skeptical and am certainly inclined to agree with Spanos on his
- point that Spiegel has misread his data.
- As for the issue of hypnotic susceptibility tests, I am not
- alone in finding them of limited usefulness and with little or no
- predictive validity, i.e., they do not predict who is and who is
- not hypnotizable. Some individuals who have scored very low on
- the Stanford scales proved to be some of my best hypnotic
- subjects. What the Stanford and other scales measure is not
- hypnotizability per se but compliance and suggestibility, e.g.,
- raising and lowering of an arm, eye closure, swaying, etc., which
- are components of but not the total of the social situation we
- call hypnosis. Response expectancies play a major and important
- role in the hypnotic situation as well as motivation. No matter
- what their score on a hypnotic susceptibility scale, people who
- have a strong need to be hypnotized in order to gain some end such
- as losing weight or stopping smoking will prove to be ideal
- subjects in the clinic. Because most clinical patients can easily
- be hypnotized in much less time than one could administer any of
- the susceptibility tests, few clinicians bother with them (Cohen,
- 1986). Moreover, even if the tests showed the client was at the
- bottom of the scale, the clinician would still be forced to find
- some way of successfully hypnotizing his patient. If one method
- doesn't work, the skilled clinician uses another technique. And,
- as all of them know, there are few--if any--unhypnotizable
- clients. If you want to use a test, one of the simplest as well
- as quickest of all and one that has as much predictive validity as
- any of the standard ones is this: approach the client, smile, and
- stick out your hand. If the client takes it and shakes it, he or
- she is socially conditioned to respond to your subsequent request
- to relax, close their eyes, and focus on the internal imagery your
- suggestions provide. This is all hypnosis is and I have yet to
- meet the human being incapable of doing this. In other words:
- everyone is hypnotizable if they wish to be and no one is
- hypnotizable if they don't want to be. This is an easily
- observable fact, despite what any and all experts may claim.
- Finally, in Cohen's words,
- Although there may be some positive correlation between
- hypnotizability and certain therapeutic gains, the reverse does
- not hold true. That is, there is no indication that low
- hypnotizability means that a given individual will not respond
- therapeutically. In my opinion, this is the major reason that
- most clinicians do not use the tests...I know of no clinician,
- including those who have developed or espoused tests, who would
- advocate withholding hypnosis from a patient simply because he or
- she scored low on a hypnotizability test. (p. 97)
-
- References
- Baker, Robert A. (1990) _They Call It Hypnosis_. Buffalo, N.Y.:
- Prometheus Books.
- Cohen, Sheldon (1986) "Clinical Usefulness of Hypnotizability
- Tests," in _Hypnosis: Questions and Answers_, ed. by Bernie
- Zilbergeld, M.G. Edelstein, and Daniel Araoz, N.Y.: W.W. Norton
- and Co.
- Coons, Philip, Milstein, Victor, and Marley, Carma (1982) "EEG
- Studies of Two Multiple Personalities and a Control," _Arch. Gen.
- Psychiatry_ 39:823-825.
- Davies, Paul (1988) "Some Considerations of the Physiological
- Effect of Hypnosis," in _Hypnosis: Current Clinical, Experimental,
- and Forensic Practices_, ed. by M. Heap, London: Croom Helm.
- Lippard, Jim (1991) "Book Review: _They Call It Hypnosis_ by
- Robert A. Baker," _The Arizona Skeptic_ 5(July/August):6-7.
- Negley-Parker, Esther (1986) "Physiological Correlates and Effects
- of Hypnosis," in _Hypnosis: Questions and Answers_, op. cit.
- Spanos, Nicholas (1986) "Hypnotic Behavior: A Social Psychological
- Interpretation of Amnesia, Analgesia, and 'Trance Logic',"
- _Behavioral and Brain Sciences_ 9:449-502.
- Spiegel, David, Cutcomb, S., Ren, C., and Pribram, K. (1985)
- "Hypnotic Hallucination Alters Evoked Potentials," _Journal of
- Abnormal Psychology_ 94:249-255.
- Thigpen, Corbett H. and Cleckley, Hervey M. (1957) _The Three
- Faces of Eve_. N.Y.: McGraw-Hill.
- Thomas, Gordon (1989) _Journey into Madness: The True Story of
- Secret CIA Mind Control and Medical Abuse_. N.Y.: Bantam Books.
-
- _Robert A. Baker has taught psychology at Stanford, MIT, and the
- University of Kentucky. He is the author of_ They Call It
- Hypnosis _and (forthcoming)_ Hidden Memories: Voices and Visions
- from Within_, both from Prometheus Books._
-
- _The Arizona Skeptic_ may be obtained via subscription for $12.50/year
- from the Phoenix Skeptics, P.O. Box 62792, Phoenix, AZ 85082-2792.
- It may also be obtained free of charge via download from GEnie's
- PSI-NET Round Table (library area 11), from the Cleveland Freenet, or
- by email from lippard@ccit.arizona.edu. Issues available in this
- format are vol. 5 nos. 1-6 and vol. 6 nos. 1-3. Also available is
- an index to all issues (vols. 1-6), by author and by subject (about
- 62K, most of which is the subject index).
-
- The following is an (incomplete) bibliography from a dormant work-
- in-progress titled "Hypnosis, Voluntary Acts, and the Law."
- Baker, Robert A. (1990) They Call It Hypnosis. Prometheus Books.
- Barber, Theodore X. (1961) "Antisocial and Criminal Acts Induced
- by Hypnosis: A Review of Experimental and Clinical Findings,"
- Archives of General Psychiatry 5:301-312.
- Barber, Theodore X., Spanos, Nicholas P., and Chaves, John F.
- (1974) Hypnosis, Imagination, and Human Potentialities.
- Pergamon.
- Coe, XX (1977) "XXX," Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
- 296:x-y.
- Feynman, Richard (1985) "Surely You're Joking, Mr. Feynman!":
- Adventures of a Curious Character. W.W. Norton.
- Goss, Michael (1991) "The Eyes Have It," Fortean Times
- #58(July):36.
- Hilgard, Ernest R. (1977) Divided Consciousness: Multiple Controls
- in Human Thought and Action. John Wiley & Sons.
- Hoencamp, Erik (1990) "Sexual Abuse and the Abuse of Hypnosis in
- the Therapeutic Relationship," International Journal of
- Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 38:283-297.
- Katz, Leo (1987) Bad Acts and Guilty Minds: Conundrums of the
- Criminal Law. University of Chicago Press.
- Levitt, Eugene E. (1977) "Research Strategies in Evaluating the
- Coercive Power of Hypnosis," Annals of the New York Academy
- of Sciences 296:86-89.
- Levitt, Eugene E., Baker, Elgan L., Jr., and Fish, Ronald C.
- (1990) "Some Conditions of Compliance and Resistance Among
- Hypnotic Subjects," American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis
- 32(April):225-236.
- Marks, John (1979) The Search for the "Manchurian Candidate": The
- CIA and Mind Control. Times Books.
- Milgram, Stanley (1974) Obedience to Authority: An Experimental
- View. Harper & Row.
- Orne, Martin T. (1972) "Can A Hypnotized Subject Be Compelled To
- Carry Out Otherwise Unacceptable Behavior? A Discussion,"
- International Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 20:101-117.
- Orne, Martin T. and Evans, Frederick J. (1965) "Social Control in
- the Psychological Experiment: Antisocial Behavior and
- Hypnosis," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
- 1:189-200.
- Rowland, Loyd W. (1939) "Will Hypnotized Persons Try To Harm
- Themselves or Others?" Journal of Abnormal and Social
- Psychology 34:114-117.
- Spanos, Nicholas P. (1986a) "Hypnotic Behavior: A Social-
- Psychological Interpretation of Amnesia, Analgesia, and
- 'Trance Logic'," Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9:449-502.
- --- (1986b) "Hypnosis, Nonvolitional Responding and Multiple
- Personality: A Social Psychological Analysis," in Brendan A.
- Maher and Winifred B. Maher (editors), Progress in
- Experimental Personality Research, vol. 14, pp. 1-62.
- Academic Press.
- Thomas, Gordon (1989) Journey into Madness: The True Story of
- Secret CIA Mind Control and Medical Abuse. N.Y.: Bantam.
- Wells, P.C. (1954) "Antisocial Uses of Hypnosis," in L.M. LeCron
- (editor), Experimental Hypnosis, pp. 376-409. Macmillan.
-
- Jim Lippard Lippard@CCIT.ARIZONA.EDU
- Dept. of Philosophy Lippard@ARIZVMS.BITNET
- University of Arizona
- Tucson, AZ 85721
-