home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
- Comments: Gated by NETNEWS@AUVM.AMERICAN.EDU
- Path: sparky!uunet!paladin.american.edu!auvm!VAXF.COLORADO.EDU!POWERS_W
- X-Envelope-to: CSG-L@vmd.cso.uiuc.edu
- X-VMS-To: @CSG
- MIME-version: 1.0
- Content-transfer-encoding: 7BIT
- Message-ID: <01GSB833WC2A005476@VAXF.COLORADO.EDU>
- Newsgroups: bit.listserv.csg-l
- Date: Mon, 14 Dec 1992 15:53:32 -0700
- Sender: "Control Systems Group Network (CSGnet)" <CSG-L@UIUCVMD.BITNET>
- From: "William T. Powers" <POWERS_W%FLC@VAXF.COLORADO.EDU>
- Subject: Neurological research
- Lines: 171
-
- [From Bill Powers (921214.1045)]
-
- Mark Olson (921214.1010) --
-
- You may have less frustration in sending if you set your right
- margin to 65 and be sure you have a hard carriage return at the
- end of each line. If you send a long string without a return you
- can overrun a buffer in your mainframe, which will cause the
- transmission to be rejected. Remember, mainframe software was not
- made to serve people; people were made to serve mainframe
- software.
- -------------------------------------------------------
- Your point about using information about the brain is a good one.
- I did a good deal of that in writing BCP. In fact when I went to
- work at the VA Research Hospital in Chicago, I was full of
- ambitions, with the Northwestern University Medical School (and
- library) right across the street. I thought good, I can just go
- through all the neurological literature and look up what the
- various parts of the brain do, and build the model around that. I
- was soon disillusioned. Maybe the information was there
- somewhere, but if so it was like a card index that someone had
- dumped on the floor, stirred, and put back at random. You'll find
- some neurological references in BCP, but not nearly as many of
- them as I had hoped to accumulate. I did make a note about Hubel
- and Weisel. Basically, however, my ambitious neurological project
- was a bust.
-
- As you say, brain researchers have found a good deal of
- interesting material without using PCT, more in recent years with
- improvements of technology. The problem is not with the
- technology, however. It's with the concepts of behavior against
- which neurological findings are compared.
-
- When you stop to think about it, neurological findings are ALWAYS
- based on SOME theory of behavior. Without any theory of behavior,
- all you have is a record of lesions in various parts of the brain
- and some recordings of spikes and potentials from electrodes in
- anatomically, but not functionally, known areas. You see the
- theory of behavior not in the findings about the brain, but in
- the descriptions of external correlates of brain activity.
-
- This problem has not escaped brain researchers. Mary brought home
- a book on lesion research recently: Damasio, H. & Damasio, A. R.;
- _Lesion Analysis in Neurophysiology_ (New York: Oxford University
- Press,1989). In the introduction, D&D say
-
- "Naturally, the lesion method can only be as good as the finest
- level of cognitive characterization and anatomical resolution it
- uses. In other words, the method's yield is limited by:
- 1. The sophistication of the neurophysiological testing or
- experimentation with which anatomical lesions are correlated.
- 2. The sophistication of the theoretical constructs and
- hypotheses being tested by the lesion probes.
- 3. The degree of sophistication with which the nervous tissue
- is conceptualized ... 4. The anatomical resolution of the methods used." (p.
- 9)
-
- From a modeler's point of view, the sophistication of
- neurophysiological testing and experimentation is not very high.
- In fact, evaluations of what is wrong with the behavior of a
- person with a brain lesion tends to rely on subjective and rather
- crude classification of symptoms rather than models of brain
- function.
-
- A large part of testing for neurological deficits consists of
- presenting stimuli to patients and recording how they respond. If
- you hold up a card with a picture, or point to an object, can the
- subject utter its name? If you tell the subject to point to your
- finger, then the subject's own nose, then the finger again and so
- forth, do the movements seem normal, and are the end-points
- accurately located? Do the movements seem retarded or uneven? Are
- there tremors or oscillations? Can the patient repeat back a
- spoken sentence, read a written sentence, follow spoken or
- written directions to do something? Can the subject name colors
- correctly, in all parts of the visual field? Can the patient
- speak the correct name of a seen person? Can the patient sing
- when so commanded? Is reading speed impaired? Is grammar correct?
- Can the patient understand and/or generate sentences of normal
- length? In general, is the response competent and appropriate to
- the stimulus?
-
- And so on and so on. What's going on is nothing more than an
- informal assessment of superficial aspects of behavior to see if
- the patient can do all the things that normal people do, and in
- the familiar way. An atmosphere of formality is generated by
- using Latin terms -- alexia for inability to read, prosopagnosia
- for inability to recognize persons, constructional apraxia for
- inability to make well-formed sentences (I think). If you strip
- away the Latin, what you have left is just a subjective
- description of what the person can't accomplish that normal
- people can. The main thing the Latin terms do is to allow you to
- say that the person "has" alexia, "has" prosopagnosia, etc.
-
- These deficits give some clues as to what various parts of the
- brain accomplish. But they don't tell us what those parts of the
- brain DO -- that is, what functions are carried out in these
- particular networks that normally result in the externally
- visible consequences that we call reading, pointing, naming, and
- so forth.
-
- Such reports of what's wrong are analogous to the report a
- technologically naive person makes to an auto mechanic: "it makes
- a funny noise sometimes; it pulls to the left; the acceleration
- is sluggish above 30 miles per hour; the steering wheel shakes."
- When the mechanic sets out to fix the problem, he doesn't look
- for a funny noise or a pull to the left or a center of
- sluggishness or a steering-wheel shake. Those are just the
- symptoms, outcomes, consequences. The mechanic understands how
- the car works, so he looks for a hole in the exhaust pipe, a
- tight wheel bearing, a malfunctioning carburetor, or anunbalanced tire. He
- doesn't say, "Oh, you car has odd-noisia, or
- dextromobilia, or accelerotardia, or manipulo-oscillia" and go
- look up the recommended treatment in a big thick book. He reasons
- out what might underlie the symptoms on the basis of the theory
- of operation of an automobile, and that theory tells him what is
- REALLY wrong with the car. That's what a good theory of behavior
- does for you, when it's tied to the actual functions of the
- device. It lets you reason your way to the layers of organization
- that underly superficial symptoms.
-
- Even the interpretation of neural connections themselves is
- conditioned by the background theory:
-
- "In the new approach, subjects' behavioral responses are not just
- linked to the stimuli that eventually triggered them, but are
- connected to mind processes and representations that handled the
- stimulus and generated the responses according to some mechanism.
- The investigations no longer shy away from formulating hypotheses
- about those mechanisms and attempting to test their validity,
- indirectly, by measuring external responses." (p. 14).
-
- The brain is a maze of connections. If you are under the
- impression that stimuli cause responses, you will select a path
- through this maze that connects inputs to outputs, and ignore all
- other connections (even while noting, in passing, that they
- exist). You'll stick an electrode somewhere the in the middle of
- this network and present a stimulus to the senses and look for a
- response from the electrode. Then you'll try to stimulate that
- spot and see what response results. If you get a response you'll
- say that you've traced out an S-R path from input to output. If a
- reference signal entered into this path somewhere, you'd never
- find it.
-
- There's no way NOT to apply a model when you're doing brain
- research. The normal accepted model is an S-R model. This way of
- thinking about behavior limits what brain research can discover
- -- or at least which of its discoveries it can purport to make
- some sense of.
-
- A neurologist who understood PCT would do different kinds of
- investigations and see different meanings in the findings. Most
- certainly, such an investigator would use different methods of
- assessing deficits. The investigator would be trying to find out
- not just what this person can control and can't control, but how
- the parameters of control have changed from those shown in normal
- people. Quantitative experiments would replace informal
- subjective classifications of symptoms. Perception and action
- would not be considered separately, but as a closed-loop system.
- The whole approach to diagnosis would change. Instead of just
- going through lists of behaviors that a normal person should be
- able to produce, the researcher would be building up a picture of
- the functions that enable such superficially-observable behaviors
- to be seen. The specific quantitative deficits would point to
- specific underlying brain functions like perception, comparison,
- and output that would produce such symptoms if operating in a
- particular defective way.
- I think that PCT would revolutionize neuropsychological research.
- -----------------------------------------------
- Best,
-
- Bill P.
-