home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
- <text id=92TT0126>
- <title>
- Jan. 20, 1992: Sizing Up The Sexes
- </title>
- <history>
- TIME--The Weekly Newsmagazine--1992
- Jan. 20, 1992 Why Are Men and Women Different?
- </history>
- <article>
- <source>Time Magazine</source>
- <hdr>
- SCIENCE, Page 42
- COVER STORIES
- Sizing Up The Sexes
- </hdr><body>
- <p>Scientists are discovering that gender differences have as much
- to do with the biology of the brain as with the way we are raised
- </p>
- <p>By Christine Gorman--Reported by J. Madeleine Nash/Los Angeles
- </p>
- <qt>
- <l>What are little boys made of?</l>
- <l>What are little boys made of?</l>
- <l>Frogs and snails</l>
- <l>And puppy dogs' tails,</l>
- <l>That's what little boys are made of.</l>
-
- <l>What are little girls made of?</l>
- <l>What are little girls made of?</l>
- <l>Sugar and spice</l>
- <l>And all that's nice,</l>
- <l>That's what little girls are made of.</l>
- </qt>
- <p>-- Anonymous
- </p>
- <p> Many scientists rely on elaborately complex and costly
- equipment to probe the mysteries confronting humankind. Not
- Melissa Hines. The UCLA behavioral scientist is hoping to solve
- one of life's oldest riddles with a toybox full of police cars,
- Lincoln Logs and Barbie dolls. For the past two years, Hines and
- her colleagues have tried to determine the origins of gender
- differences by capturing on videotape the squeals of delight,
- furrows of concentration and myriad decisions that children from
- 2 1/2 to 8 make while playing. Although both sexes play with all
- the toys available in Hines' laboratory, her work confirms what
- most parents (and more than a few aunts, uncles and
- nursery-school teachers) already know. As a group, the boys
- favor sports cars, fire trucks and Lincoln Logs, while the girls
- are drawn more often to dolls and kitchen toys.
- </p>
- <p> But one batch of girls defies expectations and
- consistently prefers the boy toys. These youngsters have a rare
- genetic abnormality that caused them to produce elevated levels
- of testosterone, among other hormones, during their embryonic
- development. On average, they play with the same toys as the
- boys in the same ways and just as often. Could it be that the
- high levels of testosterone present in their bodies before birth
- have left a permanent imprint on their brains, affecting their
- later behavior? Or did their parents, knowing of their disorder,
- somehow subtly influence their choices? If the first explanation
- is true and biology determines the choice, Hines wonders, "Why
- would you evolve to want to play with a truck?"
- </p>
- <p> Not so long ago, any career-minded researcher would have
- hesitated to ask such questions. During the feminist revolution
- of the 1970s, talk of inborn differences in the behavior of men
- and women was distinctly unfashionable, even taboo. Men
- dominated fields like architecture and engineering, it was
- argued, because of social, not hormonal, pressures. Women did
- the vast majority of society's child rearing because few other
- options were available to them. Once sexism was abolished, so
- the argument ran, the world would become a perfectly equitable,
- androgynous place, aside from a few anatomical details.
- </p>
- <p> But biology has a funny way of confounding expectations.
- Rather than disappear, the evidence for innate sexual
- differences only began to mount. In medicine, researchers
- documented that heart disease strikes men at a younger age than
- it does women and that women have a more moderate physiological
- response to stress. Researchers found subtle neurological
- differences between the sexes both in the brain's structure and
- in its functioning. In addition, another generation of parents
- discovered that, despite their best efforts to give baseballs
- to their daughters and sewing kits to their sons, girls still
- flocked to dollhouses while boys clambered into tree forts.
- Perhaps nature is more important than nurture after all.
- </p>
- <p> Even professional skeptics have been converted. "When I
- was younger, I believed that 100% of sex differences were due
- to the environment," says Jerre Levy, professor of psychology
- at the University of Chicago. Her own toddler toppled that
- utopian notion. "My daughter was 15 months old, and I had just
- dressed her in her teeny little nightie. Some guests arrived,
- and she came into the room, knowing full well that she looked
- adorable. She came in with this saucy little walk, cocking her
- head, blinking her eyes, especially at the men. You never saw
- such flirtation in your life." After 20 years spent studying the
- brain, Levy is convinced: "I'm sure there are biologically based
- differences in our behavior."
- </p>
- <p> Now that it is O.K. to admit the possibility, the search
- for sexual differences has expanded into nearly every branch of
- the life sciences. Anthropologists have debunked Margaret
- Mead's work on the extreme variability of gender roles in New
- Guinea. Psychologists are untangling the complex interplay
- between hormones and aggression. But the most provocative, if
- as yet inconclusive, discoveries of all stem from the pioneering
- exploration of a tiny 3-lb. universe: the human brain. In fact,
- some researchers predict that the confirmation of innate
- differences in behavior could lead to an unprecedented
- understanding of the mind.
- </p>
- <p> Some of the findings seem merely curious. For example,
- more men than women are lefthanded, reflecting the dominance of
- the brain's right hemisphere. By contrast, more women listen
- equally with both ears while men favor the right one.
- </p>
- <p> Other revelations are bound to provoke more controversy.
- Psychology tests, for instance, consistently support the notion
- that men and women perceive the world in subtly different ways.
- Males excel at rotating three-dimensional objects in their head.
- Females prove better at reading emotions of people in
- photographs. A growing number of scientists believe the
- discrepancies reflect functional differences in the brains of
- men and women. If true, then some misunderstandings between the
- sexes may have more to do with crossed wiring than
- cross-purposes.
- </p>
- <p> Most of the gender differences that have been uncovered so
- far are, statistically speaking, quite small. "Even the largest
- differences in cognitive function are not as large as the
- difference in male and female height," Hines notes. "You still
- see a lot of overlap." Otherwise, women could never read maps
- and men would always be lefthanded. That kind of flexibility
- within the sexes reveals just how complex a puzzle gender
- actually is, requiring pieces from biology, sociology and
- culture.
- </p>
- <p> Ironically, researchers are not entirely sure how or even
- why humans produce two sexes in the first place. (Why not just
- one--or even three--as in some species?) What is clear is
- that the two sexes originate with two distinct chromosomes.
- Women bear a double dose of the large X chromosome, while men
- usually possess a single X and a short, stumpy Y chromosome. In
- 1990 British scientists reported they had identified a single
- gene on the Y chromosome that determines maleness. Like some
- kind of biomolecular Paul Revere, this master gene rouses a
- host of its compatriots to the complex task of turning a fetus
- into a boy. Without such a signal, all human embryos would
- develop into girls. "I have all the genes for being male except
- this one, and my husband has all the genes for being female,"
- marvels evolutionary psychologist Leda Cosmides, of the
- University of California at Santa Barbara. "The only difference
- is which genes got turned on."
- </p>
- <p> Yet even this snippet of DNA is not enough to ensure a
- masculine result. An elevated level of the hormone testosterone
- is also required during the pregnancy. Where does it come from?
- The fetus' own undescended testes. In those rare cases in which
- the tiny body does not respond to the hormone, a genetically
- male fetus develops sex organs that look like a clitoris and
- vagina rather than a penis. Such people look and act female. The
- majority marry and adopt children.
- </p>
- <p> The influence of the sex hormones extends into the nervous
- system. Both males and females produce androgens, such as
- testosterone, and estrogens--although in different amounts.
- (Men and women who make no testosterone generally lack a
- libido.) Researchers suspect that an excess of testosterone
- before birth enables the right hemisphere to dominate the brain,
- resulting in lefthandedness. Since testosterone levels are
- higher in boys than in girls, that would explain why more boys
- are southpaws.
- </p>
- <p> Subtle sex-linked preferences have been detected as early
- as 52 hours after birth. In studies of 72 newborns, University
- of Chicago psychologist Martha McClintock and her students
- found that a toe-fanning reflex was stronger in the left foot
- for 60% of the males, while all the females favored their
- right. However, apart from such reflexes in the hands, legs and
- feet, the team could find no other differences in the babies'
- responses.
- </p>
- <p> One obvious place to look for gender differences is in the
- hypothalamus, a lusty little organ perched over the brain stem
- that, when sufficiently provoked, consumes a person with rage,
- thirst, hunger or desire. In animals, a region at the front of
- the organ controls sexual function and is somewhat larger in
- males than in females. But its size need not remain constant.
- Studies of tropical fish by Stanford University neurobiologist
- Russell Fernald reveal that certain cells in this tiny region
- of the brain swell markedly in an individual male whenever he
- comes to dominate a school. Unfortunately for the piscine pasha,
- the cells will also shrink if he loses control of his harem to
- another male.
- </p>
- <p> Many researchers suspect that, in humans too, sexual
- preferences are controlled by the hypothalamus. Based on a study
- of 41 autopsied brains, Simon LeVay of the Salk Institute for
- Biological Studies announced last summer that he had found a
- region in the hypothalamus that was on average twice as large
- in heterosexual men as in either women or homosexual men.
- LeVay's findings support the idea that varying hormone levels
- before birth may immutably stamp the developing brain in one
- erotic direction or another.
- </p>
- <p> These prenatal fluctuations may also steer boys toward
- more rambunctious behavior than girls. June Reinisch, director
- of the Kinsey Institute for Research in Sex, Gender and
- Reproduction at Indiana University, in a pioneering study of
- eight pairs of brothers and 17 pairs of sisters ages 6 to 18
- uncovered a complex interplay between hormones and aggression.
- As a group, the young males gave more belligerent answers than
- did the females on a multiple-choice test in which they had to
- imagine their response to stressful situations. But siblings who
- had been exposed in utero to synthetic antimiscarriage hormones
- that mimic testosterone were the most combative of all. The
- affected boys proved significantly more aggressive than their
- unaffected brothers, and the drug-exposed girls were much more
- contentious than their unexposed sisters. Reinisch could not
- determine, however, whether this childhood aggression would
- translate into greater ambition or competitiveness in the adult
- world.
- </p>
- <p> While most of the gender differences uncovered so far seem
- to fall under the purview of the hypothalamus, researchers have
- begun noting discrepancies in other parts of the brain as well.
- For the past nine years, neuroscientists have debated whether
- the corpus callosum, a thick bundle of nerves that allows the
- right half of the brain to communicate with the left, is larger
- in women than in men. If it is, and if size corresponds to
- function, then the greater crosstalk between the hemispheres
- might explain enigmatic phenomena like female intuition, which
- is supposed to accord women greater ability to read emotional
- clues.
- </p>
- <p> These conjectures about the corpus callosum have been hard
- to prove because the structure's girth varies dramatically with
- both age and health. Studies of autopsied material are of
- little use because brain tissue undergoes such dramatic changes
- in the hours after death. Neuroanatomist Laura Allen and
- neuroendocrinologist Roger Gorski of UCLA decided to try to
- circumvent some of these problems by obtaining brain scans from
- live, apparently healthy people. In their investigation of 146
- subjects, published in April, they confirmed that parts of the
- corpus callosum were up to 23% wider in women than in men. They
- also measured thicker connections between the two hemispheres
- in other parts of women's brains.
- </p>
- <p> Encouraged by the discovery of such structural
- differences, many researchers have begun looking for dichotomies
- of function as well. At the Bowman Gray Medical School in
- Winston-Salem, N.C., Cecile Naylor has determined that men and
- women enlist widely varying parts of their brain when asked to
- spell words. By monitoring increases in blood flow, the
- neuropsychologist found that women use both sides of their head
- when spelling while men use primarily their left side. Because
- the area activated on the right side is used in understanding
- emotions, the women apparently tap a wider range of experience
- for their task. Intriguingly, the effect occurred only with
- spelling and not during a memory test.
- </p>
- <p> Researchers speculate that the greater communication
- between the two sides of the brain could impair a woman's
- performance of certain highly specialized visual-spatial tasks.
- For example, the ability to tell directions on a map without
- physically having to rotate it appears stronger in those
- individuals whose brains restrict the process to the right
- hemisphere. Any crosstalk between the two sides apparently
- distracts the brain from its job. Sure enough, several studies
- have shown that this mental-rotation skill is indeed more
- tightly focused in men's brains than in women's.
- </p>
- <p> But how did it get to be that way? So far, none of the
- gender scientists have figured out whether nature or nurture is
- more important. "Nothing is ever equal, even in the beginning,"
- observes Janice Juraska, a biopsychologist at the University of
- Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. She points out, for instance, that
- mother rats lick their male offspring more frequently than they
- do their daughters. However, Juraska has demonstrated that it
- is possible to reverse some inequities by manipulating
- environmental factors. Female rats have fewer nerve connections
- than males into the hippocampus, a brain region associated with
- spatial relations and memory. But when Juraska "enriched'' the
- cages of the females with stimulating toys, the females
- developed more of these neuronal connections. "Hormones do
- affect things--it's crazy to deny that," says the researcher.
- "But there's no telling which way sex differences might go if
- we completely changed the environment." For humans, educational
- enrichment could perhaps enhance a woman's ability to work in
- three dimensions and a man's ability to interpret emotions. Says
- Juraska: "There's nothing about human brains that is so stuck
- that a different way of doing things couldn't change it
- enormously."
- </p>
- <p> Nowhere is this complex interaction between nature and
- nurture more apparent than in the unique human abilities of
- speaking, reading and writing. No one is born knowing French,
- for example; it must be learned, changing the brain forever.
- Even so, language skills are linked to specific cerebral
- centers. In a remarkable series of experiments, neurosurgeon
- George Ojemann of the University of Washington has produced
- scores of detailed maps of people's individual language centers.
- </p>
- <p> First, Ojemann tested his patients' verbal intelligence
- using a written exam. Then, during neurosurgery--which was
- performed under a local anesthetic--he asked them to name
- aloud a series of objects found in a steady stream of
- black-and-white photos. Periodically, he touched different parts
- of the brain with an electrode that temporarily blocked the
- activity of that region. (This does not hurt because the brain
- has no sense of pain.) By noting when his patients made
- mistakes, the surgeon was able to determine which sites were
- essential to naming.
- </p>
- <p> Several complex sexual differences emerged. Men with lower
- verbal IQs were more likely to have their language skills
- located toward the back of the brain. In a number of women,
- regardless of IQ, the naming ability was restricted to the
- frontal lobe. This disparity could help explain why strokes that
- affect the rear of the brain seem to be more devastating to men
- than to women.
- </p>
- <p> Intriguingly, the sexual differences are far less
- significant in people with higher verbal IQs. Their language
- skills developed in a more intermediate part of the brain. And
- yet, no two patterns were ever identical. "That to me is the
- most important finding," Ojemann says. "Instead of these sites
- being laid down more or less the same in everyone, they're laid
- down in subtly different places." Language is scattered randomly
- across these cerebral centers, he hypothesizes, because the
- skills evolved so recently.
- </p>
- <p> What no one knows for sure is just how hardwired the brain
- is. How far and at what stage can the brain's extraordinary
- flexibility be pushed? Several studies suggest that the junior
- high years are key. Girls show the same aptitudes for math as
- boys until about the seventh grade, when more and more girls
- develop math phobia. Coincidentally, that is the age at which
- boys start to shine and catch up to girls in reading.
- </p>
- <p> By one account, the gap between men and women for at least
- some mental skills has actually started to shrink. By looking
- at 25 years' worth of data from academic tests, Janet Hyde,
- professor of psychology and women's studies at the University
- of Wisconsin at Madison, discovered that overall gender
- differences for verbal and mathematical skills dramatically
- decreased after 1974. One possible explanation, Hyde notes, is
- that "Americans have changed their socialization and educational
- patterns over the past few decades. They are treating males and
- females with greater similarity."
- </p>
- <p> Even so, women still have not caught up with men on the
- mental-rotation test. Fascinated by the persistence of that gap,
- psychologists Irwin Silverman and Marion Eals of York University
- in Ontario wondered if there were any spatial tasks at which
- women outperformed men. Looking at it from the point of view of
- human evolution, Silverman and Eals reasoned that while men may
- have developed strong spatial skills in response to evolutionary
- pressures to be successful hunters, women would have needed
- other types of visual skills to excel as gatherers and foragers
- of food.
- </p>
- <p> The psychologists therefore designed a test focused on the
- ability to discern and later recall the location of objects in
- a complex, random pattern. In series of tests, student
- volunteers were given a minute to study a drawing that contained
- such unrelated objects as an elephant, a guitar and a cat. Then
- Silverman and Eals presented their subjects with a second
- drawing containing additional objects and told them to cross out
- those items that had been added and circle any that had moved.
- Sure enough, the women consistently surpassed the men in giving
- correct answers.
- </p>
- <p> What made the psychologists really sit up and take notice,
- however, was the fact that the women scored much better on the
- mental-rotation test while they were menstruating. Specifically,
- they improved their scores by 50% to 100% whenever their
- estrogen levels were at their lowest. It is not clear why this
- should be. However, Silverman and Eals are trying to find out
- if women exhibit a similar hormonal effect for any other visual
- tasks.
- </p>
- <p> Oddly enough, men may possess a similar hormonal response,
- according to new research reported in November by Doreen Kimura,
- a psychologist at the University of Western Ontario. In her
- study of 138 adults, Kimura found that males perform better on
- mental-rotation tests in the spring, when their testosterone
- levels are low, rather than in the fall, when they are higher.
- Men are also subject to a daily cycle, with testosterone levels
- lowest around 8 p.m. and peaking around 4 a.m. Thus, says June
- Reinisch of the Kinsey Institute: "When people say women can't
- be trusted because they cycle every month, my response is that
- men cycle every day, so they should only be allowed to negotiate
- peace treaties in the evening."
- </p>
- <p> Far from strengthening stereotypes about who women and men
- truly are or how they should behave, research into innate sexual
- differences only underscores humanity's awesome adaptability.
- "Gender is really a complex business," says Reinisch. "There's
- no question that hormones have an effect. But what does that
- have to do with the fact that I like to wear pink ribbons and
- you like to wear baseball gloves? Probably something, but we
- don't know what."
- </p>
- <p> Even the concept of what an innate difference represents
- is changing. The physical and chemical differences between the
- brains of the two sexes may be malleable and subject to change
- by experience: certainly an event or act of learning can
- directly affect the brain's biochemistry and physiology. And so,
- in the final analysis, it may be impossible to say where nature
- ends and nurture begins because the two are so intimately
- linked.
- </p>
-
- </body></article>
- </text>
-
-